Is the richer, the happier? Moderating roles of personality-related brain connectivity | Well-Being Sciences Review

Is the richer, the happier? Moderating roles of personality-related brain connectivity

Authors

  • Fangqi Shu
  • Jiabao Xiong
  • Jing Lin
  • Yue Zhang
  • Siyang Luo Sun Yat-sen University

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.54844/wsr.2024.0551

Keywords:

psychological well-being, personality, income, functional connectivity, human connectome project, moderating analysis

Abstract

Cultivated through the ages, the pursuit of psychological well-being (PWB) has long intrigued humanity, with prior research often examining its correlation with income. Nonetheless, predicting happiness based solely on income remains a challenge. Recent studies have highlighted personality traits as crucial moderators, exhibiting significant regulatory effects. This study seeks to explore how personality traits modulate the impact of individual income on PWB. Going beyond traditional personality trait scores, neural activity offers a multidimensional perspective on personality traits. Therefore, we utilize functional connectivity as variables reflecting personality traits in moderation analyses. Leveraging large-scale resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data, our findings underscore the pivotal moderating role of personality trait-related functional connectivity. Specifically, connections associated with the cerebellum, particularly those involving the cerebellum's neural activity, play a crucial role. This research enriches our comprehension of the relationship between income and well-being and delves into the neural mechanisms underlying the moderating influence of personality traits.

References

Boyce, C. J., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Personality and the marginal utility of income: personality interacts with increases in household income to determine life satisfaction. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 78(1-2), 183-191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.01.004

Brissenden, J. A., & Somers, D. C. (2019). Cortico-cerebellar networks for visual attention and working memory. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 239-247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.05.003

Brooks, D., Hulst, H. E., de Bruin, L., Glas, G., Geurts, J. J. G., & Douw, L. (2020). The multilayer network approach in the study of personality neuroscience. Brain Sci, 10(12), E915. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120915

Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The Quality of American Life: Perceptions, Evaluations, and Satisfactions (pp. 583). Russell Sage Foundation.

Chang, Y., & Ma, J. (2011). [The Relationship Between Graduate Students' Personality Traits, Money Attitudes, and Happiness]. Journal of Shenyang Normal University (Social Science Edition), 35, 4-7. https://doi.org/10.19496/j.cnki.ssxb.2011.02.003

Chen, X., Cai, Z., He, J., & Fan, X. (2020). Gender differences in life satisfaction among children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Happiness Studies, 21(6), 2279-2307. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-019-00169-9

Clark, A. E. (2017). Happiness, income and poverty. International Review of Economics, 64(2), 145-158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12232-017-0274-7

Clark, A. E., d'Ambrosio, C., & Ghislandi, S. (2013). Poverty and Well-Being: Panel Evidence from Germany. Paris Jourdan Sciences Economiques, 2013, ffhal-00814659f.

Clingingsmith, D. (2016). Negative emotions, income, and welfare: causal estimates from the PSID. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 130, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2016.07.004

Connor-Smith, J. K., & Flachsbart, C. (2007). Relations between personality and coping: a meta-analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol, 93(6), 1080-1107. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1080

Dai, P., Xiong, T., Zhou, X., Ou, Y., Li, Y., Kui, X., Chen, Z., Zou, B., Li, W., Huang, Z., & Consortium, T. R. (2022). The alterations of brain functional connectivity networks in major depressive disorder detected by machine learning through multisite rs-fMRI data. Behavioural Brain Research, 435, 114058. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114058

de Dreu, C. K. W., & van Lange, P. A. M. (1995). The impact of social value orientations on negotiator cognition and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1178-1188. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672952111006

DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience. Brain structure and the big five. Psychol Sci, 21(6), 820-828.10.1177/0956797610370159

Diener, E., Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will Money Increase Subjective Well-Being? Social Indicators Research, 57(2), 119-169. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014411319119

Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising income and the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267-276. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030487

Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In Nations and Households in Economic Growth (pp. 89-125). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-205050-3.50008-7

Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 27(1), 35-47. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(95)00003-B

Edwards, A. L. (1957). The Social Desirability Variable in Personality Assessment and Research (Volume viii, pp. 108). Dryden Press.

Fan, L., Duan, Q., & Luo, S. (2021a). Self-dependent neural variability predicts recovery from depressive symptoms. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(9), 962-971. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab050

Fan, L., Duan, Q., & Luo, S. (2021b). Symphony of well-being: harmony between neural variability and self-construal. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15, 679086. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.679086

Gill, M. (2014). Child poverty and children's subjective well-being. Child Indicators Research, 7(3), 451-472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-014-9237-7

Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S., Fischl, B., Andersson, J. L., Xu, J., Jbabdi, S., Webster, M., Polimeni, J. R., Van Essen, D. C., & Jenkinson, M. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. NeuroImage, 80, 105-124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127

Griffanti, L., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Beckmann, C. F., Auerbach, E. J., Douaud, G., Sexton, C. E., Zsoldos, E., Ebmeier, K. P., Filippini, N., MacKay, C. E., Moeller, S., Xu, J., Yacoub, E., Baselli, G., Ugurbil, K., Miller, K. L., & Smith, S. M. (2014). ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage, 95, 232-247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034

Heaven, P. C. L., Ciarrochi, J., Leeson, P., & Barkus, E. (2013). Agreeableness, conscientiousness, and psychoticism: distinctive influences of three personality dimensions in adolescence. British Journal of Psychology, 104(4), 481-494. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12002

Hildebrand, M., Wibbelink, C. J. M., & Verschuere, B. (2018). Do impression management and self-deception distort self-report measures with content of dynamic risk factors in offender samples? A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 58, 157-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2018.02.013

Hopwood, C. J., Brent Donnellan, M., Blonigen, D. M., Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., & Alexandra Burt, S. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on personality trait stability and growth during the transition to adulthood: A three-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(3), 545-556. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022409

Hussain, D. (2017). Conceptual referents, personality traits and income-happiness relationship: An empirical investigation. Eur J Psychol, 13(4), 733-748. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v13i4.1394

John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). Guilford Press.

Judge, T. A., Livingston, B. A., & Hurst, C. (2012). Do nice guys—and gals—really finish last? The joint effects of sex and agreeableness on income. J Pers Soc Psychol, 102(2), 390-407. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026021

Juhn C., & Murphy, K. M. (1997). Wage Inequality and Family Labor Supply. Journal of Labor Economics. https://doi.org/10.1086/209847

Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 107(38), https://doi.org/16489-16493.10.1073/pnas.1011492107

Kaplan, G. A., Shema, S. J., & Leite, C. M. A. (2008). Socioeconomic determinants of psychological well-being: the role of income, income change, and income sources during the course of 29 years. Annals of Epidemiology, 18(7), 531-537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2008.03.006

Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(3), 280-287. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167296223006

Khodabakhsh, S. (2022). Factors affecting life satisfaction of older adults in Asia: A systematic review. Journal of Happiness Studies, 23(3), 1289-1304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-021-00433-x

Kong, F., Hu, S., Wang, X., Song, Y., & Liu, J. (2015). Neural correlates of the happy life: the amplitude of spontaneous low frequency fluctuations predicts subjective well-being. NeuroImage, 107, 136-145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.033

Kruschwitz, J. D., Walter, M., Varikuti, D., Jensen, J., Plichta, M. M., Haddad, L., Grimm, O., Mohnke, S., Pöhland, L., Schott, B., Wold, A., Mühleisen, T. W., Heinz, A., Erk, S., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., Witt, S. H., Nöthen, M. M., Rietschel, M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., & Walter, H. (2015). 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 polymorphism and neuroticism are linked by resting state functional connectivity of amygdala and fusiform gyrus. Brain Struct Funct, 220(4), 2373-2385. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0782-0

Kunisato, Y., Okamoto, Y., Okada, G., Aoyama, S., Nishiyama, Y., Onoda, K., & Yamawaki, S. (2011). Personality traits and the amplitude of spontaneous low-frequency oscillations during resting state. Neuroscience Letters, 492(2), 109-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.067

La, B. T., Lim, S., Cameron, M. P., Tran, T. Q., & Nguyen, M. T. (2021). Absolute income, comparison income and subjective well-being in a transitional country: panel evidence from Vietnamese household surveys. Economic Analysis and Policy, 72, 368-385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eap.2021.09.008

Li, J. [A Multimodal Research of Resting-State fMRI on the Eysenck Personality Traits]. Shanxi Medical University (Master's Thesis). 2018.

Li, L., Li, L. M. W., Ma, J., Lu, A., & Dai, Z. (2023). The relationship between personality traits and well-being via brain functional connectivity. Journal of Happiness Studies, 24(6), 2127-2152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-023-00674-y

Li, R., Zhu, X., Zheng, Z., Wang, P., & Li, J. (2020). Subjective well-being is associated with the functional connectivity network of the dorsal anterior insula. Neuropsychologia, 141, https://doi.org/107393.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107393

López Ulloa, B. F., Møller, V., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2013). How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. Journal of Population Ageing, 6(3), 227-246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12062-013-9085-0

Luo, S., Liman Man Wai Li, Espina, E., Bond, M. H., Lun, V. M., Huang, L., Duan, Q., & Liu, J. H. (2023). Individual uniqueness in trust profiles and well-being: understanding the role of cultural tightness-looseness from a representation similarity perspective. British Journal of Social Psychology, 62(2), 825-844. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12599

Luo, S., Yuan, H., Wang, Y., & Bond, M. H. (2024). Culturomics: taking the cross-scale, interdisciplinary science of culture into the next decade. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 167, 105942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105942

Luo, S., Zhu, Y., & Han, S. (2022). Functional connectome fingerprint of holistic-analytic cultural style. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 17(2), 172-186. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab080

Luo, S., Zhu, Y., Xu, Y., & Kong, Q. (2017). The oxytocinergic system modulates sadistic context-dependent empathy in humans. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 12463. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-12671-2

Luo, Y., Kong, F., Qi, S., You, X., & Huang, X. (2016). Resting-state functional connectivity of the default mode network associated with happiness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 516-524. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv132

Lyon, K. A., Elliott, R., Ware, K., Juhasz, G., & Brown, L. J. E. (2021). Associations between facets and aspects of big five personality and affective disorders: A systematic review and best evidence synthesis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 288, 175-188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.03.061

Mankiw, N. G. (2021). Principles of Economics. Cengage Learning. Accessed June 23, 2024. https://thuvienso.hoasen.edu.vn/handle/123456789/11032

Matz, S. C., & Gladstone, J. J. (2020). Nice guys finish last: when and why agreeableness is associated with economic hardship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(3), 545-561. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000220

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2004). A contemplated revision of the NEO five-factor inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(3), 587-596. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00118-1

Morawetz, C., Riedel, M. C., Salo, T., Berboth, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., & Kohn, N. (2020). Multiple large-scale neural networks underlying emotion regulation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 116, 382-395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.001

Mullis, R. J. (1992). Measures of economic well-being as predictors of psychological well-being. Social Indicators Research, 26(2), 119-135. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00304395

Nostro, A. D., Müller, V. I., Varikuti, D. P., Pläschke, R. N., Hoffstaedter, F., Langner, R., Patil, K. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Predicting personality from network-based resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Structure and Function, 223(6), 2699-2719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-018-1651-z

Nyhus, E. K., & Webley, P. (2001). The role of personality in household saving and borrowing behaviour. European Journal of Personality, 15(S1), S85-S103. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.422

Passamonti, L., Terracciano, A., Riccelli, R., Donzuso, G., Cerasa, A., Vaccaro, M., Novellino, F., Fera, F., & Quattrone, A. (2015). Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. NeuroImage, 104, 301-309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.017

Peng, L., & Luo, S. (2021). Impact of social economic development on personality traits among Chinese college students: a cross-temporal meta-analysis, 2001-2016. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110461

Proto, E., & Rustichini, A. (2015). Life satisfaction, income and personality. Journal of Economic Psychology, 48, 17-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2015.02.001

Quan, F., Yang, R., Zhu, W., Wang, Y., Gong, X., Chen, Y., Dong, Y., & Xia, L. X. (2019). The relationship between hostile attribution bias and aggression and the mediating effect of anger rumination. Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 228-234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.11.029

Ryan, J. P., Sheu, L. K., & Gianaros, P. J. (2011). Resting state functional connectivity within the cingulate cortex jointly predicts agreeableness and stressor-evoked cardiovascular reactivity. NeuroImage, 55(1), 363-370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.064

Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2010). Subjective well-being, income, economic development and growth. SSRN Electronic Journal, 2010, 16441. https://doi.org/10.3386/w16441

Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2012). The new stylized facts about income and subjective well-being. Emotion, 12(6), 1181-1187. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029873

Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Douaud, G., Beckmann, C. F., Glasser, M. F., Griffanti, L., & Smith, S. M. (2014). Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage, 90, 449-468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046

Salsman, J. M., Lai, J. S., Hendrie, H. C., Butt, Z., Zill, N., Pilkonis, P. A., Peterson, C., Stoney, C. M., Brouwers, P., & Cella, D. (2014). Assessing psychological well-being: self-report instruments for the NIH toolbox. Quality of Life Research, 23(1), 205-215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-013-0452-3

Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., Sawada, R., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., & Toichi, M. (2019). Resting-state neural activity and connectivity associated with subjective happiness. Scientific Reports, 9, 12098. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-48510-9

Schmutte, P. S., & Ryff, C. D. (1997). Personality and well-being: reexamining methods and meanings. J Pers Soc Psychol, 73(3), 549-559.10.1037//0022-3514.73.3.549

Schurer, S., & Yong, J. (2012). Personality, well-being and the marginal utility of income: What can we learn from random coefficient models. Te Herenga Waka—Victoria University of Wellington. Retrieved February 29, 2024, from https://ir.wgtn.ac.nz/handle/123456789/18617

Sengupta, N. K., Osborne, D., Houkamau, C. A., Hoverd, W. J., Wilson, M. S., Halliday, L. M., WestNewman, T., Barlow, F. K., Armstrong, G., Robertson, A., & Sibley, C. G. (2012). How much happiness does money buy? Income and subjective well-being in New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 41(2), 21-34.

Soto, C. J., & Luhmann, M (2013). Who Can Buy Happiness : Personality Traits Moderate the Effects of Stable Income Differences and Income Fluctuations on Life Satisfaction. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(1), 46-53. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612444139

Speed, B. C., Nelson, B. D., Levinson, A. R., Perlman, G., Klein, D. N., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2018). Extraversion, neuroticism, and the electrocortical response to monetary rewards in adolescent girls. Biological Psychology, 136, 111-118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.05.017

Sugiura, Y., & Sugiura, T. (2018). Mindfulness as a moderator in the relation between income and psychological well-being. Front Psychol, 9, 1477. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01477

Syrén, S. M., Kokko, K., Pulkkinen, L., & Pehkonen, J. (2020). Income and mental well-being: personality traits as moderators. Journal of Happiness Studies, 21(2), 547-571. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-019-00076-z

Tran, Y., Craig, A., Boord, P., Connell, K., Cooper, N., & Gordon, E. (2006). Personality traits and its association with resting regional brain activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60(3), 215-224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.05.008

Uchida, Y., & Ogihara, Y. (2012). Personal or interpersonal construal of happiness: a cultural psychological perspective. International Journal of Wellbeing, 354-369. https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v2.i4.5

Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C., Dalton, K. M., Mueller, C. J., Rosenkranz, M. A., Ryff, C. D., Singer, B. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Making a life worth living: neural correlates of well-being. Psychol Sci, 15(6), 367-372. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00686.x

van den Heuvel, M. P., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(8), 519-534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008

Van Essen, D. C., Ugurbil, K., Auerbach, E., Barch, D., Behrens, T. E. J., Bucholz, R., Chang, A., Chen, L., Corbetta, M., Curtiss, S. W., Della Penna, S., Feinberg, D., Glasser, M. F., Harel, N., Heath, A. C., Larson-Prior, L., Marcus, D., Michalareas, G., Moeller, S., …, & Yacoub, E. (2012). The Human Connectome Project: A data acquisition perspective. NeuroImage, 62(4), 2222-2231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018

Veenhoven, R. (2012). Happiness: Also known as "life satisfaction" and "subjective well-being". In K. Land, A. Michalos, & M. Sirgy, (Eds.), Handbook of Social Indicators and Quality of Life Research (pp, 63-77). Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2421-1_3

Waris, O., Soveri, A., Lukasik, K. M., Lehtonen, M., & Laine, M. (2018). Working memory and the big five. Personality and Individual Differences, 130, 26-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.027

Watson, D. (2004). Stability versus change, dependability versus error: issues in the assessment of personality over time. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(4), 319-350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.03.001

Watson, D. C. (2014). Materialism: Profiles of agreeableness and neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 197-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.014

Wei, L., Duan, X., Yang, Y., Liao, W., Gao, Q., Ding, J. R., Zhang, Z., Zeng, W., Li, Y., Lu, G., & Chen, H. (2011). The synchronization of spontaneous BOLD activity predicts extraversion and neuroticism. Brain Research, 1419, 68-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.060

Yang, D., Zheng, G., Wang, H., & Li, M. (2022). Education, income, and happiness: evidence from China. Front Public Health, 10, 855327. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2022.855327

Yasmeen, G., & Ejaz, A. (2021). The impact of economic resources on psychological wellbeing of families in Pakistan case study: low income families in Peshawar khyber paktunkhawa. International Journal of Business, Economics and Law, 17(3), 42-48.

Ye, M., Zhang, J., & Li, H. (2023). Twins, income, and happiness: Evidence from China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United of States of America, 120(25), e2221884120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2221884120

Yuan, H., & Luo, S.Y. (2024). Representation similarity analysis-A new perspective to study sociocultural change: taking the mental health of elderly people as an example. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(7), 938-972.

Yu, Z., & Chen, L. (2016). Income and well-being: Relative income and absolute income weaken negative emotion, but only relative income improves positive emotion. Front Psychol, 7, 2012. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02012

Zhu, Z., Qin, S., Dodd, A., & Conti, M. (2023). Understanding the relationships between emotion regulation strategies and Big Five personality traits for supporting effective emotion regulation tools/interventions design. Advanced Design Research, 1(1), 38-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijadr.2023.06.001

Additional Files

Published

2025-04-30

How to Cite

1.
Shu F, Xiong J, Lin J, Zhang Y, Luo S. Is the richer, the happier? Moderating roles of personality-related brain connectivity. WSR. 2025;1(1):59-70. doi:10.54844/wsr.2024.0551

Issue

Section

Original Articles

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Is the richer, the happier? Moderating roles of personality-related brain connectivity


Fangqi Shu#, Jiabao Xiong#, Jing Lin#, Yue Zhang#, Siyang Luo*

Department of Psychology, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Social Cognitive Neuroscience and Mental Health, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Brain Function and Disease, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou 510006, Guangdong Province, China


#These authors contributed equally to this work

*Corresponding Author:

Siyang Luo, Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, 132 Univ. Town Waihuan E Rd, Panyu District, Guangzhou 510006, Guangdong Province, China. Email: ljc520ida@163.com/luosy6@mail.sysu.edu.com; https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1067-7560


Received: 29 February 2024 Revised: 1 August 2024 Accepted: 22 November 2024


ABSTRACT

Cultivated through the ages, the pursuit of psychological well-being (PWB) has long intrigued humanity, with prior research often examining its correlation with income. Nonetheless, predicting happiness based solely on income remains a challenge. Recent studies have highlighted personality traits as crucial moderators, exhibiting significant regulatory effects. This study seeks to explore how personality traits modulate the impact of individual income on PWB. Going beyond traditional personality trait scores, neural activity offers a multidimensional perspective on personality traits. Therefore, we utilize functional connectivity as variables reflecting personality traits in moderation analyses. Leveraging large-scale resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data, our findings underscore the pivotal moderating role of personality trait-related functional connectivity. Specifically, connections associated with the cerebellum, particularly those involving the cerebellum's neural activity, play a crucial role. This research enriches our comprehension of the relationship between income and well-being and delves into the neural mechanisms underlying the moderating influence of personality traits.

Key words: psychological well-being, personality, income, functional connectivity, human connectome project, moderating analysis

INTRODUCTION

What constitutes the source of psychological well-being (PWB) and happiness According to the "Living well" theory (Salsman et al., 2014), PWB can be categorized into two broad components: affective experiences and cognitive evaluations. These components encompass three key dimensions:(1) positive affect (affective experiences), (2) life satisfaction, and (3) meaning and purpose (cognitive evaluations). The concepts of PWB and happiness significantly overlap. Ruut Veenhoven posits that happiness is a positive emotional experience derived from a meaningful life and subjective life satisfaction (Veenhoven, 2012). It involves personal positive emotions and life satisfaction but focuses primarily on hedonic aspects without emphasizing life's meaning and goals. In contrast, PWB not only includes aspects of happiness but also places significant importance on meaning and purpose.

While a commonly held belief asserts that "Money can't buy happiness", paradoxically, divergent views prevail in social discourse (Clark, 2017). Some contend that "Money is akin to a smoothing iron capable of alleviating life's tribulations (Sacks et al., 2010)". In academic circles, the discourse surrounding the relationship between PWB and money remains pervasive and contentiou (Diener et al., 2013; Sacks et al., 2012; Sengupta et al., 2012).

Income is the primary source through which people acquire money. According to Mankiw's "Principles of Economics", Individuals and households receive income through many sources like labor and investment, and this income is usually held in the form of money, which can be in cash or bank accounts (Mankiw, 2021). For instance, previous studies have explored the correlation between income and PWB. In previous research, income has been found to predict all PWB dimensions, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally (Kaplan et al., 2008). PWB has been found to have a significant but weak positive correlation with income (correlation coefficients generally ranging from 0.10 to 0.20) (Mullis, 1992; Sugiura & Sugiura, 2018; Yasmeen & Ejaz, 2021). In a study on lower-middle-class families in Turkey, income was significantly and moderately positively correlated with all dimensions of PWB (correlation coefficients ranging from 0.303 to 0.605) (Yasmeen & Ejaz, 2021). Some Studies have also explored the correlation between income and happiness. A comprehensive study spanning 24 years across 19 countries and three continents revealed a distinct positive correlation between income and happiness within nations (Easterlin, 1974). Other studies also supported this positive correlation between income and happiness. That is, income can positively predict happiness (La et al., 2021; Yang et al., 2022; Ye et al., 2023). A twin study conducted in China involving 750 pairs of identical twins and 456 pairs of fraternal twins demonstrated a significant positive correlation between income and happiness. Doubling income resulted in a 0.26-unit increase in happiness scores, equivalent to a 0.37 standard deviation increase (Ye et al., 2023). The above evidence indicates a possible positive correlation between PWB and income, laying the groundwork for the formulation of our hypothesis.

Nevertheless, income is one facet within the multifaceted framework contributing to subjective well-being (López Ulloa et al., 2013) (includes overall life satisfaction, experiencing positive emotions, and a reduction in negative emotions [Sugiura & Sugiura, 2018]), happiness (Uchida & Ogihara, 2012), life satisfaction (Chen et al., 2020; Khodabakhsh, 2022), and PWB (Campbell et al., 1976). A paradox emerges in the relationship between PWB and income. A large-scale study on quality of life indicates that some high-income families report low levels of PWB. In contrast, some low-income families report high levels of PWB (Campbell et al., 1976). Income augmentation also does not consistently portend a concomitant rise in subjective well-being. Renowned scholar Easterlin posited the "Easterlin paradox" grounded in his research discoveries: Despite the exponential growth observed in per capita income over time, the parallel trajectory of happiness exhibits relative stability (Easterlin, 1995). This phenomenon may stem from the intricate nature of real-life experiences, where happiness is influenced not solely by one factor but by the complicated interplay among multiple factors, as mentioned earlier (Fan et al., 2021a; Luo et al., 2022). The above study results demonstrate the potentially complex relationship between PWB and income and possible moderating variables, which warrant further investigation.

Previous studies have indicated that personality traits can modulate the relationship between income and happiness (Chang & Ma, 2011; Proto & Rustichini, 2015; Soto, 2013; Syrén et al., 2020). Empirical investigations conducted by scholars Proto and Rustichini have revealed the nuanced moderating role of neuroticism in shaping the relationship between income and life satisfaction. The correlation between income and life satisfaction exhibits heightened strength among individuals with lower income and higher neuroticism. In contrast, it diminishes among those with higher income and elevated neurotic tendencies. For those with high income, individuals with higher levels of neuroticism derive less satisfaction from additional income compared to those with lower levels of neuroticism. Conversely, for those with low income, individuals with higher levels of neuroticism derive more satisfaction from additional income than those with lower levels of neuroticism. When considering the interaction between income and neuroticism, income alone does not significantly impact (Proto & Rustichini, 2015).

Syrén's team has also elucidated the role of personality traits as moderators in the intricate nexus between income and mental well-being (Syrén et al., 2020). Specifically, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism exhibit adverse moderation effects on the relationship between monthly income and mental well-being, while openness demonstrates a positive moderation effect. Individuals with higher neuroticism scores tend to experience heightened life satisfaction with increased income. Since income is the primary source through which people acquire money, the investigation of the attitude towards money and personality can show the relationship between income and personality. In a complementary study, Chang and Ma delineated the intricate interplay of personality traits as partial mediators in the predictive linkages between overall happiness and attitudes toward money (Chang & Ma, 2011). Neuroticism emerges as a significant positive predictor of dimensions such as distrust and money anxiety within money attitudes, whereas extraversion emerges as a negative predictor of the dimension of distrust. Concurrently, the maintenance-reservation dimension within money attitudes positively correlates with happiness, while the dimension of distrust exhibits a negative association with happiness (Chang & Ma, 2011).

Having established the intricate interplay between personality traits and their moderating or mediating effects on the relationship between income and well-being, it is crucial to delve into the neurobiological underpinnings accompanying these observed phenomena (Kong et al., 2015; Urry et al., 2004). According to previous studies, it is evident that there are many shared neural pathways between personality traits and well-being, such as the insula, precuneus, amygdala, thalamus, and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) (Kruschwitz et al., 2015; Li, 2018; Li et al., 2020; Luo et al., 2016; Passamonti et al., 2015; Ryan et al., 2011; Sato et al., 2019; Wei et al., 2011). Through mediation analysis, it was found that the average functional connectivity and global network efficiency can mediate the effect of personality traits on PWB (Kong et al., 2015), and thalamic and insular connectivity significantly mediated the association between neuroticism and eudaimonic well-being (Li et al., 2023). These findings underscore the complexity of how personality traits influence PWB, indicating that this relationship cannot be adequately captured solely by simple personality trait scores. Moreover, personality assessment through self-report questionnaires is susceptible to biases such as social desirability, impression management, and self-deception (Hildebrand et al., 2018). Even with consistent core items, variations in questionnaire wording and format can impact the stability of personality measurements (Watson, 2004). Therefore, using more objective measures, such as neuroimaging methods, not only provides explanatory rather than descriptive theories for personality traits (DeYoung et al., 2010) but also avoids the effects of social desirability, impression management, and self-deception.

Previous studies have found that the relationship between personality traits and PWB is influenced by numerous complex cognitive functions (Kunisato et al., 2011; Salsman et al., 2014; Waris et al., 2018). For instance, neuroticism may impact well-being through self-consciousness, emotional awareness, cognitive control, performance monitoring, and autobiographical memory (Kong et al., 2015; Li et al., 2023). These mental activities require effective information exchange between different brain regions, and the interactions among these cognitive processes are crucial for understanding the relationship between personality traits and PWB. Functional connectivity reflects the level of communication between brain regions, indicating the information exchange and interaction among different brain areas (Luo et al., 2022; van den Heuvel & Hulshoff Pol, 2010). Therefore, employing functional connectivity to represent personality traits is likely to provide deeper insights into the intricate relationship between personality and PWB.

Therefore, the primary objective of this study is to analyze functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data corresponding to personality traits to systematically explore the potential moderating effects of personality variables on the intricate relationship between income and well-being. As highlighted earlier, the rationale for selecting neurophysiological characterization data in this research is grounded in its capacity to comprehensively elucidate the biological foundations of personality traits.

Based on the evidence above, we posit the following hypotheses: (1) Neuroticism and income interact: Among individuals with stronger functional connections associated with neuroticism, the positive predictive effect of income on PWB is heightened (Hussain, 2017; Speed et al., 2018). (2) Extraversion and income interact: Within groups exhibiting enhanced functional connections linked to extraversion, the adverse impact of income reduction on PWB is diminished compared to groups characterized by weaker functional connections associated with extraversion (Hussain, 2017; Speed et al., 2018).

This investigation is poised to establish a foundation for delving into the potential neurobiological underpinnings of personality, thereby providing crucial cognitive neuroscientific support for forthcoming research on personality traits. Consequently, this study holds considerable scientific reference value and is positioned to make a meaningful contribution to future research endeavors within this domain (Easterlin, 1974). Furthermore, using fMRI data as quantitative metrics for personality variables offers a distinct advantage in minimizing errors compared to self-reported data, thereby mitigating potential biases arising from social desirability and rater biases (Edwards, 1957). Additionally, we used household income in our analysis because it offers several advantages over personal income. Fluctuations in income among family members can balance each other out, providing a more stable measure of economic status. Many economic and social phenomena, such as consumption behavior, savings, and educational expenses, are related to household units. Therefore, using household income allows for more accurate analysis and prediction of these phenomena (Mankiw, 2021). Moreover, household income helps maintain consistency and comparability across statistical data, providing a standardized way to measure the economic status of households. This enhances the reliability and comparability of various financial data and analyses (Juhn & Murphy, 1997). Consequently, we chose household income for our analysis.

METHOD

Overview

In our analytical approach, we initially utilized each connectivity strength as a moderator variable in predicting happiness based on income. We identified edges that demonstrated significant moderating effects and established significant correlations with personality traits. Subsequently, weights were assigned to edges corresponding to each personality trait, and a composite score was computed. This composite score was then employed as a moderator variable in predicting PWB based on income.

Database

All data, including income, PWB, personality traits, and neuro-imaging data, were accessed from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) 1200 Subjects Data Release (Van Essen et al., 2012). We employed pre-existing high-level resting-state functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (rs-fMRI) connectivity analyses, which included precomputed functional connectivity matrices by the HCP. The details of the imaging data analysis will be explained below. We chose household income as the representation of monetary resources. Income levels were quantified using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) and encoded into eight categories: < $10,000 = 1; 10,000-19,999 = 2; 20,000-29,999 = 3; 30,000-39,999 = 4; 40,000-49,999 = 5; 50,000-74,999 = 6; 75,000-99,999 = 7; ≥ 100,000 = 8. Personality traits were assessed using the five-factor model and measured by the Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI), which has demonstrated excellent reliability and validity (McCrae & Costa, 2004). The scale includes "general life satisfaction", "meaning and purpose", and "positive affect". General life satisfaction primarily measures satisfaction with various aspects of life (e.g., "I am satisfied with my life"), meaning and purpose encompass several related yet distinct elements, such as coherence in life, perceiving life as valuable and meaningful, engaging in personally fulfilling activities, and having a sense of purpose. Positive affect is divided into three factors: the first factor reflects high activation emotions (e.g., "I felt joyful"), the second factor includes low activation emotions (e.g., "I felt peaceful"), and the third factor involves cognitive engagement (e.g., "I felt attentive") (Salsman et al., 2014). Participants self-reported their PWB through the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Toolbox PWB surveys (http://www.nihtoolbox.org). Previous research has shown correlations between overall life satisfaction, meaning and purpose, and positive effect on income (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2002; Kahneman & Deaton, 2010; Kasser & Ryan, 1996). Therefore, The PWB score in our study was calculated as the sum of the scores from these three dimensions. It is important to note that all data used in this study were obtained in compliance with the HCP guidelines and ethical standards.

All studies were approved by the ethics committee of Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University. Written informed consent was provided by each participant before they took part in the HCP and this project was approved by the Institutional Review Board for ethical research.

Imaging data processing

The dataset consisted of data from all 1003 subjects, each with four complete rs-fMRI runs. The preprocessing pipeline utilized was the minimally-preprocessing pipeline (Glasser et al., 2013), coupled with artifact removal using Independent Component Analysis followed by the FMRIB's Expert Analysis Tool (ICA+FIX) (Griffanti et al., 2014; Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2014). Following preprocessing, the data underwent group Principal Component Analysis (PCA) and were fed into group Independent Component Analysis (ICA).

During this process, spatial ICA maps were overlaid onto each subject's rs-fMRI time series data to extract a representative time series for each ICA component. Treating each ICA component as a network "node", a partial temporal correlation was computed between the nodes' time series. The correlation values were subsequently transformed from Pearson correlation scores (R-values) to Z-statistics. These steps collectively yielded the average functional connectivity matrix for each subject. We choose 300 distinct ICA components as our materials. Interested readers can refer to the official HCP website for more detailed information.

Functional connectivity matrix analysis

We excluded nine subjects with incomplete data from the analysis. Subsequently, we employed each edge as a moderator variable representing the correlation between two nodes. Income was considered the independent variable in the prediction of PWB. Our objective was to identify edges with significant moderating effects, and false discovery rate (FDR) correction was applied to adjust P-values, with a threshold set at P = 0.01. However, no edge survived the correction. The entire moderator analysis was conducted using a linear model.

In light of the absence of edges meeting the correction criteria, the data using uncorrected P-values that were below the threshold. After identifying significant moderating edges, Pearson correlations were computed between these edges and the five personality traits. To ensure the robustness of our findings, P-values underwent correction for multiple comparisons using FDR, with a threshold set at P = 0.01. Unfortunately, no edge met the criteria for significance after correction. Next, we used the functional connection of P < 0.01 to represent the personality trait for subsequent analyses and calculations (not corrected by FDR).

Statistical analysis

Our statistical approach comprises two steps. First, for each personality trait, PCA was employed to ascertain the weights of each edge representing the personality trait. Then, based on the weights, we obtain the functional connectivity scores for each personality trait of each subject. Specifically, the functional connectivity scores were calculated by the weighted average of each edge. Utilizing these functional connectivity scores, we predicted the personality trait scores of the subjects and obtained significant results. In the second step, the model presented in Equation (1) was employed to test our hypotheses. This model incorporates the main and interaction effects between household income and the neural functional network of the five personality traits: Yi = α0 + α1Mi + β0Xi + β1Mi × Xi + ε, (1).

Where Y represents the PWB score of participants, X denotes household income, M is the moderator variable, and M × X represents the interaction term between the moderator and income. The moderating variables utilized were participants' neural functional connectivity scores for each personality trait: Neuroticism, Extroversion, Openness, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness. We examined whether the effect of household income on the PWB of subjects is moderated by these personality traits using linear regression. All independent variables were standardized to mitigate multicollinearity. A logarithmic transformation was applied to the PWB score. The goodness-of-fit was assessed using R2. All estimations were conducted using SPSS 27.0.

RESULTS

Functional connectivity matrix analysis

After the analysis, we identified 2563 edges exhibiting significant moderating effects. Within this subset, we observed significant correlations with personality traits: 4 edges were significantly correlated with agreeableness (including three positively correlated edges and one negatively correlated edge), three edges with conscientiousness (one positive and two negative), five edges with extroversion (two positive and three negative), ten edges with neuroticism (three positive and seven negative), and three edges with openness (two positive and one negative), Detailed information is available in Figure 1 and Table S1-S5. Screening out the most relevant edges of personality traits among the vast number of functional connections will help us construct a representative neural functional network of personality traits. The weighted average of these edges was performed to obtain the functional connectivity scores of the corresponding personality traits (Table S6-S10), and these functional connectivity scores will be used as our moderating variables for regression analysis in the following studies (See the Methods section for details).

Figure 1

Figure 1. Edges with significant moderating effects related to personality traits. The functional connectivity results indicate that highlighted regions exhibit similar activation patterns. For each personality trait, we displayed two edges with the largest absolute weights in the figure, while additional edges can be found in the supplementary materials. The brain cross-section images are sourced from the official HCP dataset.

Statistical analysis result

Table 1 summarizes descriptive statistics of moderator variables used in the study. We observed significant interaction effects between income and the neural function network of Neuroticism (β = 0.013, t = 6.403, P < 0.001). In addition to the regression results, we present the interaction effects in Figure 2 and Table 2 to facilitate the interpretation of moderation. Simple slope analyses indicated a positive correlation between income and PWB for individuals with a relatively high level of neuroticism (whose functional connectivity score was one standard deviation [SD] above the mean), β = 0.029, P < 0.001 (Table S11). In contrast, there was no significant relationship between income and PWB for individuals with a relatively low level of neuroticism (1 SD below the mean), β = 0.000, P = 0.867. The plotted lines illustrate the support for Hypothesis 1, suggesting that the PWB of individuals with high levels of neuroticism is more susceptible to the influence of income.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Interaction effects plot (Income × Neuroticism). The vertical axis represents the PWB score of participants, calculated as the logarithm of the sum of life satisfaction, meaning and purpose, and positive affect scores. The horizontal axis depicts the household income of participants, categorized as follows: <$10,000 = 1, 10k-19,999 = 2, 20k-29,999 = 3, 30k-39,999 = 4, 40k-49,999 = 5, 50k-74,999 = 6, 75k-99,999 = 7, ≥100,000 = 8. The asterisks (***) indicate that the positive relationship between income and PWB was significant only for participants with medium or high neuroticism levels (P < 0.01).

Table 1: Descriptive statistics of participants (N = 994)
Variable Description M SD Min Max
PWB score The sum of life satisfaction score, meaning and purpose score and positive affect score 157.09 21.67 81.40 217.80
Household income Total household income* 5.09 2.14 1.00 8.00
Age Age in years 28.74 3.70 22 37
Gender 0-female, 1-male 0.47 0.50 0 1
NEOFAC_N NEO- FFI Personality Neuroticism 16.40 7.33 0 43
NEOFAC_O NEO- FFI Personality Openness 28.46 6.22 10 47
NEOFAC_E NEO- FFI Personality Extroversion 30.72 5.99 10 47
NEOFAC_C NEO- FFI Personality Conscientiousness 34.46 5.94 11 48
NEOFAC_A NEO- FFI Personality Agreeableness 33.58 5.79 10 48
* < $10,000 = 1, 10k-19,999 = 2, 20k-29,999 = 3, 30k-39,999 = 4, 40k-49,999 = 5, 50k-74,999 = 6, 75k-99,999 = 7, >= 100,000 =8. M, mean score of variables; SD, standard deviation of variables; Min, minimum values; Max, maximumindicate values; NEOFAC, Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Five-Factor Inventory_Neuroticism; NEOFAC_O, Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Five-Factor Inventory_Openness; NEOFAC_E, Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Five-Factor Inventory_Extroversion; NEOFAC_C, Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Five-Factor Inventory_Conscientiousness; NEOFAC_A, Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Five-Factor Inventory_Agreeableness.
Table 2: Regression analysis result (N = 994)
Model Variable Coefficient SE t P
1 (Constant) 5.047 0.004 1170.007 <0.001
Income 0.015 0.002 7.206 <0.001
FC- N -0.019 0.004 -4.343 <0.001
FC- E 0.022 0.009 2.483 0.013
FC- O 0.009 0.004 1.973 0.049
FC- C 0.008 0.004 1.768 0.077
FC- A 0.001 0.004 0.255 0.799
2 (Constant) 5.046 0.004 1219.379 <0.001
Income 0.015 0.002 7.907 <0.001
FC- N -0.014 0.004 -3.265 0.001
FC- E 0.019 0.008 2.206 0.028
FC- O 0.006 0.004 1.378 0.169
FC- C 0.006 0.004 1.525 0.128
FC- A 0.002 0.004 0.385 0.700
FC- N* Income 0.013 0.002 6.403 <0.001
FC- E* Income -0.016 0.004 -3.890 <0.001
FC- O* Income -0.007 0.002 -3.559 <0.001
FC- C* Income -0.005 0.002 -2.411 0.016
FC- A* Income -0.003 0.002 -1.579 0.115
We employed two models to predict PWB scores. In Model 1, only the level of income and functional connectivity (FC), which is related to personality scores, were used as independent variables (IV). FC-N, functional connectivity of Neuroticism; FC-E, FC of Extraversion; FC-O, FC of Openness; FC-C, FC of Conscientiousness; FC-A, FC of Agreeableness. In Model 2, we included the interaction between FC and income as an additional IV. SE, standard error.

The interaction effect between income and the neural function network of Extroversion was significant β = -0.016, t = -3.890, P < 0.001), Simple slope analyses indicated that income was positively correlated with PWB for individuals with relatively low levels of extroversion, β = 0.036, P < 0.001. In contrast, there was no relationship between income and PWB for individuals with relatively high levels of extroversion, β = -0.007, P = 0.150. Plot lines illustrate the support for Hypothesis 2, stating that the PWB of individuals with low levels of extroversion is more likely to be affected by income (Figure 3).

Figure 3

Figure 3. Interaction effects plot (Income × Extroversion). Simple slopes analyses indicate a positive correlation between income and PWB for individuals with relatively low levels of extroversion. The asterisks (***) indicate that the positive relationship between income and PWB was significant only for participants with medium or low extroversion levels (P < 0.01).

We also observed a significant interaction effect between income and the neural function network of Openness (Figure 4) and an interaction effect between income and the neural function network of Conscientiousness (Figure 5). These results suggest that individuals with high levels of openness and conscientiousness are less likely to have their PWB affected by income. The interaction effect between income and the neural function network of Agreeableness is insignificant, illustrating no moderation effect of Agreeableness.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Interaction effects plot (Income × Openness). Simple slopes analyses indicate a positive correlation between income and PWB for individuals with relatively low levels of openness. The asterisks (***) indicate that the positive relationship between income and PWB was significant only for participants with medium or low openness levels (P < 0.01).

Figure 5

Figure 5. Interaction effects plot (Income × Conscientiousness). The asterisks (*** /*) indicate that the positive relationship between income and PWB significant for participants with low, medium or high conscientiousness levels (P < 0.01/ P < 0.05).

DISCUSSION

This study aims to investigate the impact of income on PWB and the moderating effect of personality traits, which are indexed by brain connectivity. The results reveal that individuals with heightened levels of neuroticism demonstrate a heightened susceptibility of their PWB to income fluctuations. Specifically, under conditions of low household income, individuals characterized by high neuroticism report diminished PWB compared to those with low neuroticism. Additionally, our findings suggest that individuals with lower levels of extroversion are more susceptible to the impact of income on their PWB. Conversely, individuals characterized by heightened levels of openness and conscientiousness exhibit greater resilience, as their PWB appears less influenced by income fluctuations. Previous literature has established that individual personality traits influence emotional regulation strategies and cognitive processes, thereby impacting PWB (Morawetz et al., 2020). Specifically, household income is inversely correlated with negative emotions (Clingingsmith, 2016; Yu & Chen, 2016). When individuals experience negative emotions triggered by poverty, their emotional regulation and cognitive coping strategies—modulated by personality traits—can have varying effects on well-being (Zhu et al., 2023). Research indicates that individuals with high levels of neuroticism tend to be more prone to worry, depression, emotional instability, self-indulgence, jealousy, and dissatisfaction (John & Srivastava, 1999), and they may also encounter greater financial difficulties (Nyhus & Webley, 2001). In contrast, individuals with high levels of extraversion, openness, and conscientiousness typically employ positive coping strategies when facing challenges. Those high in extraversion and openness are more inclined to seek external support and cultivate harmonious interpersonal relationships, which reduces their susceptibility to anxiety (Connor-Smith & Flachsbart, 2007). It can be inferred that individuals with high levels of neuroticism are more likely to experience negative emotions and distress due to low income and a tendency toward debt. Conversely, individuals with high levels of extraversion, openness, and conscientiousness are more likely to seek constructive solutions and maintain positive interpersonal relationships, even in the face of low income. This proactive approach allows them to gain social support and positive emotions from others, thereby mitigating the adverse effects of low income. Therefore, it is evident that individuals with different personality traits experience varying levels of PWB when confronted with low income, which further supported our results.

Our study unveils distinct neural connections that modulate the relationship between income, PWB, and their correlation with personality traits. Specifically, connections linking the superior parietal lobule to the posterior cerebellum and the orbitofrontal cortex to the anterior cerebellum emerge as pivotal in moderating the impact of neuroticism on income and PWB. Furthermore, connectivity between the central lobule and the occipital lobe and connections between the midbrain and the occipital lobe is deemed significant when extroversion moderates. Additionally, our findings underscore the importance of connections between the thalamus and the occipital lobe, as well as between the dorsal prefrontal lobe and the cerebellum, in modulating the influence of openness. Moreover, the connectivity linking the orbitofrontal cortex to the left posterior lobe of the cerebellum emerges as a pivotal regulator associated with conscientiousness.

In our investigation, many connections linked to the cerebellum have been discerned. Previous scholarly endeavors have firmly established the cerebellum's involvement in sensorimotor functions and pivotal roles concerning emotional regulation and cognitive processing (Dai et al., 2022). Diverse activation patterns within the cerebellum have been observed across various tasks, encompassing working memory, attention, language, executive functions, and emotional processing (Brissenden & Somers, 2019). This diversity is postulated to stem from the intricate existence of cognitive functional loops between the cerebellum and the brain, facilitating extensive bidirectional communication (Kunisato et al., 2011).

However, agreeableness is the only personality trait we found that does not have a significant moderating effect. This can be attributed to two primary reasons: Firstly, the null effect of agreeableness in our results may be linked to the independent variable we used in our analysis, namely, family household income. It's crucial to note that individuals with varying levels of agreeableness within the same household share the identical household income value. This disparity in agreeableness levels amidst identical household income contexts could result in a diminished impact of household income on PWB for highly agreeable individuals, thus statistically explaining the observed null effect of agreeableness in our findings. Moreover, previous research has shown that individuals with high agreeableness are more likely to face financial difficulties (Schmutte & Ryff, 1997) because they tend to accommodate others (Watson, 2014) and are therefore more willing to sacrifice their personal resources for others (Quan et al., 2019). When facing stressful events, they actively seek external emotional support, thereby reducing anxiety (Lyon et al., 2021). Therefore, we hypothesize that household income does not significantly affect the PWB of individuals with high agreeableness. Research has found that individuals with high agreeableness are more prone to experiencing financial difficulties (Matz & Gladstone, 2020) because they are more likely to accommodate others (de Dreu & van Lange, 1995) and thus more willing to sacrifice their personal resources for others (Judge et al., 2012). From this, we can infer that individuals with low agreeableness are less likely to make concessions to others and, therefore, less likely to face financial difficulties. Therefore, highly agreeable individuals are more likely to make concessions to other family members, reducing their own benefits. At the same time, those with low agreeableness can better secure more benefits for themselves. This may relatively reduce the disparity in the impact of household income on the PWB of individuals with different levels of agreeableness. In conclusion, the moderating effect of agreeableness on household income and PWB may also be affected by other factors, such as family relationships, which can be further explored.

In addition, individuals with high agreeableness are characterized by trust in others, generosity, humility, and empathy (Heaven et al., 2013). Previous studies have shown that individuals with high agreeableness generally have higher levels of PWB (Schmutte & Ryff, 1997). Compared to other personality traits, agreeable individuals place less importance on money (Watson, 2014) and tend to remember and recall positive information (Quan et al., 2019). When facing stressful events, such as limited access to money, they actively seek external emotional support, thereby reducing anxiety and maintaining the current level of happiness (Ka & Lje, 2021). Therefore, this poverty resilience of highly agreeable individuals may contribute to the null effect of agreeableness observed in our analysis. In conclusion, the moderating effect of agreeableness on household income and PWB may also be influenced by other factors, such as family relationships, which warrant further exploration.

The practical ramifications of this study extend to the exploration of PWB and the measurement of personality traits through resting-state fMRI. Many individuals grapple with distress from their modest income, significantly impacting their overall PWB (Clark et al., 2013; Gill, 2014). Moreover, individuals sharing comparable income levels may manifest disparate levels of PWB, a phenomenon potentially influenced by distinct personality traits (Boyce & Wood, 2011; Schurer & Yong, 2012; Syrén et al., 2020). Our findings clarify the moderating influence of personality traits on the nexus between income and PWB. An avenue for augmenting PWB may be fostering an optimistic and open demeanor. Consequently, individuals are encouraged to contemplate personal transformation as a mechanism for enhancing PWB, transcending a sole reliance on external factors for PWB improvement.

This study discerns functional brain connections corresponding to specific personality traits and elucidates the functional networks associated with each trait, thereby introducing a novel metric for gauging personality attributes. Conventional personality assessment tools, notably questionnaires, are marred by individual subjectivity, social desirability bias, and intentional deception. Consequently, this innovative measurement approach holds the potential to mitigate errors and find versatile applications, including the evaluation of personality traits in forensic contexts.

LIMITATION

Although previous studies have found that functional connectivity can predict personality traits (Brooks et al., 2020; Nostro et al., 2018; Tran et al., 2006), it is undeniable that personality traits are complex constructs influenced by a multitude of factors, including genetics (Hopwood et al., 2011), the natural environment (Hopwood et al., 2011), and the social environment (Peng & Luo, 2021) . While functional connectivity offers valuable insights into the neural basis of personality traits, it alone cannot fully capture their intricacies. Future research should aim to integrate genetic, neural, environmental, and social factors to provide a more comprehensive understanding of personality traits. Recent studies have introduced representation similarity analysis as a novel tool to extract the degree of association between different dimensions, thereby constructing global feature patterns of object variables (Fan et al., 2021b; Luo et al., 2017; Luo et al., 2024; Yuan & Luo, 2024). Incorporating such perspectives and methodologies in future research could provide a more nuanced understanding of the complex, multi-factorial nature of personality traits.

Furthermore, our fMRI data has not undergone correction for multiple comparisons, thereby introducing the possibility of type one errors. In future research endeavors, we aspire to employ more rigorous statistical methodologies and substantiate the generalizability of our results across diverse databases.

CONCLUSION

Our empirical findings provide compelling evidence that core personality traits, including neuroticism, extraversion, openness, and conscientiousness, wield a discernible moderating influence on the intricate interplay between household income and overall PWB.

DECLARATION

Acknowledgement

We used HCP data provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657).

Author contributions

Shu FQ: Conceptualization, Resources, Data curatio, Methodology, Validation, Writing—Original draft, Writing—Review and Editing; Xiong JB: Conceptualization, Resources, Data curatio, Methodology, Validation, Writing—Original draft, Writing—Review and Editing; Lin J: Conceptualization, Resources, Data curatio, Methodology, Validation, Writing—Original draft, Writing—Review and Editing; Zhang Y: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing—Original draft, Writing—Review and Editing; Luo SY:Conceptualization, Project administration, Writing—Original draft, Writing—Review and Editing. All authors commented on the manuscript.

Ethics approval

All studies were approved by the ethics committee of Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University. Written informed consent was provided by each participant before they took part in the HCP and this project was approved by the Institutional Review Board for ethical research.

Source of funding

This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Project 32071081, 32371125) (S. L.).

Conflict of interest

Luo SY is the editorial board member of the journal. The article was subject to the journal's standard procedures, with peer review handled independently of the editor and the affiliated research groups.

Use of large language models, AI and machine learning tools

None declared.

Data availability statement

No additional data.

REFERENCES

  1. Boyce, C. J., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Personality and the marginal utility of income: personality interacts with increases in household income to determine life satisfaction. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 78(1-2), 183-191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.01.004
  2. Brissenden, J. A., & Somers, D. C. (2019). Cortico-cerebellar networks for visual attention and working memory. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 239-247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.05.003
  3. Brooks, D., Hulst, H. E., de Bruin, L., Glas, G., Geurts, J. J. G., & Douw, L. (2020). The multilayer network approach in the study of personality neuroscience. Brain Sci, 10(12), E915. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120915
  4. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The Quality of American Life: Perceptions, Evaluations, and Satisfactions (pp. 583). Russell Sage Foundation.
  5. Chang, Y., & Ma, J. (2011). [The Relationship Between Graduate Students' Personality Traits, Money Attitudes, and Happiness]. Journal of Shenyang Normal University (Social Science Edition), 35, 4-7. https://doi.org/10.19496/j.cnki.ssxb.2011.02.003
  6. Chen, X., Cai, Z., He, J., & Fan, X. (2020). Gender differences in life satisfaction among children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Happiness Studies, 21(6), 2279-2307. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-019-00169-9
  7. Clark, A. E. (2017). Happiness, income and poverty. International Review of Economics, 64(2), 145-158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12232-017-0274-7
  8. Clark, A. E., d'Ambrosio, C., & Ghislandi, S. (2013). Poverty and Well-Being: Panel Evidence from Germany. Paris Jourdan Sciences Economiques, 2013, ffhal-00814659f.
  9. Clingingsmith, D. (2016). Negative emotions, income, and welfare: causal estimates from the PSID. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 130, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2016.07.004
  10. Connor-Smith, J. K., & Flachsbart, C. (2007). Relations between personality and coping: a meta-analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol, 93(6), 1080-1107. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1080
  11. Dai, P., Xiong, T., Zhou, X., Ou, Y., Li, Y., Kui, X., Chen, Z., Zou, B., Li, W., Huang, Z., & Consortium, T. R. (2022). The alterations of brain functional connectivity networks in major depressive disorder detected by machine learning through multisite rs-fMRI data. Behavioural Brain Research, 435, 114058. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114058
  12. de Dreu, C. K. W., & van Lange, P. A. M. (1995). The impact of social value orientations on negotiator cognition and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1178-1188. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672952111006
  13. DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience. Brain structure and the big five. Psychol Sci, 21(6), 820-828.10.1177/0956797610370159
  14. Diener, E., Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will Money Increase Subjective Well-Being? Social Indicators Research, 57(2), 119-169. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014411319119
  15. Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising income and the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267-276. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030487
  16. Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In Nations and Households in Economic Growth (pp. 89-125). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-205050-3.50008-7
  17. Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 27(1), 35-47. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(95)00003-B
  18. Edwards, A. L. (1957). The Social Desirability Variable in Personality Assessment and Research (Volume viii, pp. 108). Dryden Press.
  19. Fan, L., Duan, Q., & Luo, S. (2021a). Self-dependent neural variability predicts recovery from depressive symptoms. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(9), 962-971. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab050
  20. Fan, L., Duan, Q., & Luo, S. (2021b). Symphony of well-being: harmony between neural variability and self-construal. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15, 679086. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.679086
  21. Gill, M. (2014). Child poverty and children's subjective well-being. Child Indicators Research, 7(3), 451-472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-014-9237-7
  22. Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S., Fischl, B., Andersson, J. L., Xu, J., Jbabdi, S., Webster, M., Polimeni, J. R., Van Essen, D. C., & Jenkinson, M. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. NeuroImage, 80, 105-124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127
  23. Griffanti, L., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Beckmann, C. F., Auerbach, E. J., Douaud, G., Sexton, C. E., Zsoldos, E., Ebmeier, K. P., Filippini, N., MacKay, C. E., Moeller, S., Xu, J., Yacoub, E., Baselli, G., Ugurbil, K., Miller, K. L., & Smith, S. M. (2014). ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage, 95, 232-247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034
  24. Heaven, P. C. L., Ciarrochi, J., Leeson, P., & Barkus, E. (2013). Agreeableness, conscientiousness, and psychoticism: distinctive influences of three personality dimensions in adolescence. British Journal of Psychology, 104(4), 481-494. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12002
  25. Hildebrand, M., Wibbelink, C. J. M., & Verschuere, B. (2018). Do impression management and self-deception distort self-report measures with content of dynamic risk factors in offender samples? A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 58, 157-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2018.02.013
  26. Hopwood, C. J., Brent Donnellan, M., Blonigen, D. M., Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., & Alexandra Burt, S. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on personality trait stability and growth during the transition to adulthood: A three-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(3), 545-556. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022409
  27. Hussain, D. (2017). Conceptual referents, personality traits and income-happiness relationship: An empirical investigation. Eur J Psychol, 13(4), 733-748. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v13i4.1394
  28. John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). Guilford Press.
  29. Judge, T. A., Livingston, B. A., & Hurst, C. (2012). Do nice guys—and gals—really finish last? The joint effects of sex and agreeableness on income. J Pers Soc Psychol, 102(2), 390-407. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026021
  30. Juhn C., & Murphy, K. M. (1997). Wage Inequality and Family Labor Supply. Journal of Labor Economics. https://doi.org/10.1086/209847
  31. Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 107(38), https://doi.org/16489-16493.10.1073/pnas.1011492107
  32. Kaplan, G. A., Shema, S. J., & Leite, C. M. A. (2008). Socioeconomic determinants of psychological well-being: the role of income, income change, and income sources during the course of 29 years. Annals of Epidemiology, 18(7), 531-537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2008.03.006
  33. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(3), 280-287. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167296223006
  34. Khodabakhsh, S. (2022). Factors affecting life satisfaction of older adults in Asia: A systematic review. Journal of Happiness Studies, 23(3), 1289-1304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-021-00433-x
  35. Kong, F., Hu, S., Wang, X., Song, Y., & Liu, J. (2015). Neural correlates of the happy life: the amplitude of spontaneous low frequency fluctuations predicts subjective well-being. NeuroImage, 107, 136-145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.033
  36. Kruschwitz, J. D., Walter, M., Varikuti, D., Jensen, J., Plichta, M. M., Haddad, L., Grimm, O., Mohnke, S., Pöhland, L., Schott, B., Wold, A., Mühleisen, T. W., Heinz, A., Erk, S., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., Witt, S. H., Nöthen, M. M., Rietschel, M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., & Walter, H. (2015). 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 polymorphism and neuroticism are linked by resting state functional connectivity of amygdala and fusiform gyrus. Brain Struct Funct, 220(4), 2373-2385. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0782-0
  37. Kunisato, Y., Okamoto, Y., Okada, G., Aoyama, S., Nishiyama, Y., Onoda, K., & Yamawaki, S. (2011). Personality traits and the amplitude of spontaneous low-frequency oscillations during resting state. Neuroscience Letters, 492(2), 109-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.067
  38. La, B. T., Lim, S., Cameron, M. P., Tran, T. Q., & Nguyen, M. T. (2021). Absolute income, comparison income and subjective well-being in a transitional country: panel evidence from Vietnamese household surveys. Economic Analysis and Policy, 72, 368-385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eap.2021.09.008
  39. Li, J. [A Multimodal Research of Resting-State fMRI on the Eysenck Personality Traits]. Shanxi Medical University (Master's Thesis). 2018.
  40. Li, L., Li, L. M. W., Ma, J., Lu, A., & Dai, Z. (2023). The relationship between personality traits and well-being via brain functional connectivity. Journal of Happiness Studies, 24(6), 2127-2152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-023-00674-y
  41. Li, R., Zhu, X., Zheng, Z., Wang, P., & Li, J. (2020). Subjective well-being is associated with the functional connectivity network of the dorsal anterior insula. Neuropsychologia, 141, https://doi.org/107393.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107393
  42. López Ulloa, B. F., Møller, V., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2013). How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. Journal of Population Ageing, 6(3), 227-246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12062-013-9085-0
  43. Luo, S., Liman Man Wai Li, Espina, E., Bond, M. H., Lun, V. M., Huang, L., Duan, Q., & Liu, J. H. (2023). Individual uniqueness in trust profiles and well-being: understanding the role of cultural tightness-looseness from a representation similarity perspective. British Journal of Social Psychology, 62(2), 825-844. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12599
  44. Luo, S., Yuan, H., Wang, Y., & Bond, M. H. (2024). Culturomics: taking the cross-scale, interdisciplinary science of culture into the next decade. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 167, 105942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105942
  45. Luo, S., Zhu, Y., & Han, S. (2022). Functional connectome fingerprint of holistic-analytic cultural style. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 17(2), 172-186. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab080
  46. Luo, S., Zhu, Y., Xu, Y., & Kong, Q. (2017). The oxytocinergic system modulates sadistic context-dependent empathy in humans. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 12463. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-12671-2
  47. Luo, Y., Kong, F., Qi, S., You, X., & Huang, X. (2016). Resting-state functional connectivity of the default mode network associated with happiness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 516-524. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv132
  48. Lyon, K. A., Elliott, R., Ware, K., Juhasz, G., & Brown, L. J. E. (2021). Associations between facets and aspects of big five personality and affective disorders: A systematic review and best evidence synthesis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 288, 175-188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.03.061
  49. Mankiw, N. G. (2021). Principles of Economics. Cengage Learning. Accessed June 23, 2024. https://thuvienso.hoasen.edu.vn/handle/123456789/11032
  50. Matz, S. C., & Gladstone, J. J. (2020). Nice guys finish last: when and why agreeableness is associated with economic hardship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(3), 545-561. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000220
  51. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2004). A contemplated revision of the NEO five-factor inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(3), 587-596. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00118-1
  52. Morawetz, C., Riedel, M. C., Salo, T., Berboth, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., & Kohn, N. (2020). Multiple large-scale neural networks underlying emotion regulation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 116, 382-395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.001
  53. Mullis, R. J. (1992). Measures of economic well-being as predictors of psychological well-being. Social Indicators Research, 26(2), 119-135. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00304395
  54. Nostro, A. D., Müller, V. I., Varikuti, D. P., Pläschke, R. N., Hoffstaedter, F., Langner, R., Patil, K. R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Predicting personality from network-based resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Structure and Function, 223(6), 2699-2719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-018-1651-z
  55. Nyhus, E. K., & Webley, P. (2001). The role of personality in household saving and borrowing behaviour. European Journal of Personality, 15(S1), S85-S103. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.422
  56. Passamonti, L., Terracciano, A., Riccelli, R., Donzuso, G., Cerasa, A., Vaccaro, M., Novellino, F., Fera, F., & Quattrone, A. (2015). Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. NeuroImage, 104, 301-309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.017
  57. Peng, L., & Luo, S. (2021). Impact of social economic development on personality traits among Chinese college students: a cross-temporal meta-analysis, 2001-2016. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110461
  58. Proto, E., & Rustichini, A. (2015). Life satisfaction, income and personality. Journal of Economic Psychology, 48, 17-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2015.02.001
  59. Quan, F., Yang, R., Zhu, W., Wang, Y., Gong, X., Chen, Y., Dong, Y., & Xia, L. X. (2019). The relationship between hostile attribution bias and aggression and the mediating effect of anger rumination. Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 228-234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.11.029
  60. Ryan, J. P., Sheu, L. K., & Gianaros, P. J. (2011). Resting state functional connectivity within the cingulate cortex jointly predicts agreeableness and stressor-evoked cardiovascular reactivity. NeuroImage, 55(1), 363-370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.064
  61. Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2010). Subjective well-being, income, economic development and growth. SSRN Electronic Journal, 2010, 16441. https://doi.org/10.3386/w16441
  62. Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2012). The new stylized facts about income and subjective well-being. Emotion, 12(6), 1181-1187. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029873
  63. Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Douaud, G., Beckmann, C. F., Glasser, M. F., Griffanti, L., & Smith, S. M. (2014). Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage, 90, 449-468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046
  64. Salsman, J. M., Lai, J. S., Hendrie, H. C., Butt, Z., Zill, N., Pilkonis, P. A., Peterson, C., Stoney, C. M., Brouwers, P., & Cella, D. (2014). Assessing psychological well-being: self-report instruments for the NIH toolbox. Quality of Life Research, 23(1), 205-215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-013-0452-3
  65. Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., Sawada, R., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., & Toichi, M. (2019). Resting-state neural activity and connectivity associated with subjective happiness. Scientific Reports, 9, 12098. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-48510-9
  66. Schmutte, P. S., & Ryff, C. D. (1997). Personality and well-being: reexamining methods and meanings. J Pers Soc Psychol, 73(3), 549-559.10.1037//0022-3514.73.3.549
  67. Schurer, S., & Yong, J. (2012). Personality, well-being and the marginal utility of income: What can we learn from random coefficient models. Te Herenga Waka—Victoria University of Wellington. Retrieved February 29, 2024, from https://ir.wgtn.ac.nz/handle/123456789/18617
  68. Sengupta, N. K., Osborne, D., Houkamau, C. A., Hoverd, W. J., Wilson, M. S., Halliday, L. M., WestNewman, T., Barlow, F. K., Armstrong, G., Robertson, A., & Sibley, C. G. (2012). How much happiness does money buy? Income and subjective well-being in New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 41(2), 21-34.
  69. Soto, C. J., & Luhmann, M (2013). Who Can Buy Happiness : Personality Traits Moderate the Effects of Stable Income Differences and Income Fluctuations on Life Satisfaction. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(1), 46-53. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550612444139
  70. Speed, B. C., Nelson, B. D., Levinson, A. R., Perlman, G., Klein, D. N., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2018). Extraversion, neuroticism, and the electrocortical response to monetary rewards in adolescent girls. Biological Psychology, 136, 111-118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.05.017
  71. Sugiura, Y., & Sugiura, T. (2018). Mindfulness as a moderator in the relation between income and psychological well-being. Front Psychol, 9, 1477. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01477
  72. Syrén, S. M., Kokko, K., Pulkkinen, L., & Pehkonen, J. (2020). Income and mental well-being: personality traits as moderators. Journal of Happiness Studies, 21(2), 547-571. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-019-00076-z
  73. Tran, Y., Craig, A., Boord, P., Connell, K., Cooper, N., & Gordon, E. (2006). Personality traits and its association with resting regional brain activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60(3), 215-224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.05.008
  74. Uchida, Y., & Ogihara, Y. (2012). Personal or interpersonal construal of happiness: a cultural psychological perspective. International Journal of Wellbeing, 354-369. https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v2.i4.5
  75. Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C., Dalton, K. M., Mueller, C. J., Rosenkranz, M. A., Ryff, C. D., Singer, B. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Making a life worth living: neural correlates of well-being. Psychol Sci, 15(6), 367-372. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00686.x
  76. van den Heuvel, M. P., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(8), 519-534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008
  77. Van Essen, D. C., Ugurbil, K., Auerbach, E., Barch, D., Behrens, T. E. J., Bucholz, R., Chang, A., Chen, L., Corbetta, M., Curtiss, S. W., Della Penna, S., Feinberg, D., Glasser, M. F., Harel, N., Heath, A. C., Larson-Prior, L., Marcus, D., Michalareas, G., Moeller, S., …, & Yacoub, E. (2012). The Human Connectome Project: A data acquisition perspective. NeuroImage, 62(4), 2222-2231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018
  78. Veenhoven, R. (2012). Happiness: Also known as "life satisfaction" and "subjective well-being". In K. Land, A. Michalos, & M. Sirgy, (Eds.), Handbook of Social Indicators and Quality of Life Research (pp, 63-77). Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2421-1_3
  79. Waris, O., Soveri, A., Lukasik, K. M., Lehtonen, M., & Laine, M. (2018). Working memory and the big five. Personality and Individual Differences, 130, 26-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.027
  80. Watson, D. (2004). Stability versus change, dependability versus error: issues in the assessment of personality over time. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(4), 319-350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.03.001
  81. Watson, D. C. (2014). Materialism: Profiles of agreeableness and neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 197-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.014
  82. Wei, L., Duan, X., Yang, Y., Liao, W., Gao, Q., Ding, J. R., Zhang, Z., Zeng, W., Li, Y., Lu, G., & Chen, H. (2011). The synchronization of spontaneous BOLD activity predicts extraversion and neuroticism. Brain Research, 1419, 68-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.060
  83. Yang, D., Zheng, G., Wang, H., & Li, M. (2022). Education, income, and happiness: evidence from China. Front Public Health, 10, 855327. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2022.855327
  84. Yasmeen, G., & Ejaz, A. (2021). The impact of economic resources on psychological wellbeing of families in Pakistan case study: low income families in Peshawar khyber paktunkhawa. International Journal of Business, Economics and Law, 17(3), 42-48.
  85. Ye, M., Zhang, J., & Li, H. (2023). Twins, income, and happiness: Evidence from China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United of States of America, 120(25), e2221884120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2221884120
  86. Yuan, H., & Luo, S.Y. (2024). Representation similarity analysis-A new perspective to study sociocultural change: taking the mental health of elderly people as an example. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(7), 938-972.
  87. Yu, Z., & Chen, L. (2016). Income and well-being: Relative income and absolute income weaken negative emotion, but only relative income improves positive emotion. Front Psychol, 7, 2012. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02012
  88. Zhu, Z., Qin, S., Dodd, A., & Conti, M. (2023). Understanding the relationships between emotion regulation strategies and Big Five personality traits for supporting effective emotion regulation tools/interventions design. Advanced Design Research, 1(1), 38-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijadr.2023.06.001